LOOPS:
An Informal Timbre Experiment
Robert
Erickson
Musical
structures, no matter what their particular musical dimensions, rest upon
perceptions of similarity and difference.
We know something of pitch structure, less about rhythmic structure and
almost nothing about timbre as a structural element in music. Indeed, it has commonly been thought to be a
subsidiary and/or ornamental musical characteristic, without potential for generating
structure. Nevertheless, there are
compositions by Webern, Varèse
and a few other composers which indicate that the prevailing view may be
wrong. LOOPS is
an experiment to obtain information about the question: can there be
specifically timbral organization in music.
The
notion of a structured sequence of sounds whose organization is not primarily a
matter of pitch was first proposed by Schoenberg (1911) in the final paragraphs
of his Harmonielehre.
I
cannot readily admit that there is such a difference, as is usually expressed,
between timbre and pitch. It is my opinion that the sound becomes noticeable
through its timbre and one of its dimensions is pitch. In other words: the larger realm is the timbre,
whereas the pitch is one of the smaller provinces. The pitch is nothing but timbre measured in
one direction. If it is possible to make
compositional structures from timbres which differ according to height, [pitch]
structures which we call melodies, sequences producing an effect similar to
thought, then it must also be possible to create such sequences from the
timbres of the other dimension from what we normally and simply call
timbre. Such sequences would work with
an inherent logic, equivalent to the kind of logic which is effective
in the melodies based on pitch. All this
seems a fantasy of the future, which it probably is. Yet I am firmly convinced that it can be
realized.1
But
Schoenberg composed no klangfarbenmelodie himself, and in a letter of
1951 to Rufer (1969) he discussed the idea again,
this time with a slightly different emphasis.
After remarking that "Webern's
compositions realized only the smallest part of my conception of
"klangfarbenmelodie" he goes on to say that what might appear to be
klangfarbenmelodie in his own works is always some kind of polyphony, and that
these isolated instances are not really melodies. Melodies require a constructive unity, an
organization, and "in my conception such (klangfarbenmelodie) forms had
become something new for which as yet there is no description, because, indeed,
they do not yet exist."
The
remark about polyphony is especially interesting, because it is clear that
klangfarbenmelodie must be compound, i.e., exhibit more or less easily
perceptible polyphonic characteristics, since the constituent timbres tend to
cohere on the basis of timbre class. The reason it is difficult to find
unequivocal examples of klangfarbenmelodie (in Schoenberg's 1911 sense of the term) may
therefore be related to the tendency of linear sequences to break into the
separate perceptual streams or channels which are implicit in extended melodies
as "hidden polyphony" or compound melodic line.
The
formation of perceptual streams or channels was first investigated by Miller
and Heise (1950).
They found that a rapid trill broke into two separate streams when the
frequency distance exceeded about one seventh of an octave in music
ranges. They named the region of
transition the "trill threshold". Norman (1966, 1967) commented upon the experiment of Miller and Heise, offering several possible explanations of the
phenomenon, and reported that when listeners were asked to decided whether a
probe tone followed the higher or lower of two repeating background tones, they
had little difficulty when the frequency of the probe tone was between those of
the two background tones, but when the probe tone was much higher or lower than
the background tones, the task was correspondingly more difficulty. Warren and his associates (1969) found that in
listening to a tape loop of four sounds (40 Hz. square wave, 1000Hz sine wave,
the vowel "ee", white noise burst) played
at a rate of 200 msec for each of the four items,
their subjects had great difficulty in judging the order of the sounds. The four different sounds tended to form four
separate perceptual channels unless played very slowly. Bregman and
My
LOOPS experiment is an attempt to discover whether concepts of perceptual
stream or channel can be helpful in a musical understanding of the effects of
fast discrete changes of instrumental timbre.
If a repeating melodic pattern of six pitches is performed by five
instruments, with each instrument playing a single note, in the manner of a hocket, then each pitch of the pattern will eventually be
played by a different instrument (Example 1) and one can form opinions about
the effects of the timbral versus the pitch
dimension.
Clearly
one is able to listen to this delicious confusion in more than one way: (1) one
may follow the tune through its changes of timbre; (2) one may begin to form
perceptual streams on a pitch basis (in this kind of listening the C/B flat
patterns of the high line and the E flat/D flat patterns of the low line are
clearest); (3) one may follow the line of a single instrument (marimba is easy,
clarinet is more difficult); (4) one may listen - and this is most likely - in a mixed manner, using (1) or (2) or (3),
depending upon the detailed musical situation
at any particular moment.
With
fewer instruments one might expect the channeling to be stronger, because each
instrument is heard with fewer rests between appearances. Example 2 employs three instruments at I and five instruments at J in an eight pitch melodic
pattern.
It
may be very slightly easier to follow an individual instrument line when three
instruments are employed, but not much.
Following an instrument appears to depend more upon the special
characteristics of the instrument, especially its attack quality, in relation
to the total group or subgroup. The
marimba line can be tracked almost too easily, but the other two instruments at
I are not so easy to disentangle.
If
a melodic pattern has a wider pitch ambit, Example 3, one could expect more
channeling on a pitch basis. Example 3
also uses a more complex pattern: a nine
tone sequence plus a nine tone variant (simple repeating sequences soon become
boring) with enough octave displacements to make a three octave range. A further complication here is that instead
of simply rotating the instruments in sequence they are used in patterns. Sometimes the pattern is modified because a
pitch is not available on a particular instrument. Especially interesting is a
noticeable change of grouping which must be timbre determined -at O, after the
complex pattern of N and before the complex pattern of P.
If
one lengthens notes in one of the instruments a melodic channel in the timbre of
the instrument will be formed. There is
nothing musically new or problematical in passages such as are shown in
Example 4, although perceptual channeling becomes very complex, and one is very
aware of texture and of the intensified disjunction (contrast) between the
instrumental timbres. Notice too, that
the pitch range has been further extended.
It
appears that the answer to the question, can a melodic figure be preserved while
undergoing radical changes of instrumental timbre is yes - but. The "but" includes matters such as
the total range of the melodic pattern, the tempo, the particular instruments
involved, their timbres at the specified pitches and the type of articulation
of the attack and decay. It is easy, but
allowing only slight overlaps of sound, to turn a precariously sequential
melodic formation into something clearly polyphonic.
How
strong is the effect of the timbre pattern vis a vis the melodic
pattern? Strong, but no general
statement is possible. The sub-patterns
produced by the competition between pitch channeling and timbre channeling are
local effects, but controllable, and full of compositional potential, not least
in the area of rhythm. It is of great
interest that, in spite of the meter of the counting process, the patterning is
chiefly a result of tonic accent, loudness of the various notes in an
instrument's repertory, articulation of attack and decay, etc. Timbral
distinctiveness or vividness in any micro context sounds very important,
perhaps crucial, to the formation of the sub-groupings, and, therefore, the
rhythm and the higher levels of the musical organization.
Why
is the number of effective perceptual categories often less than six, even when
all instruments are playing? The answer
to this is an interesting problem in psychoacoustics (are psychologists
listening?) but it is of musical interest too, for the confusions among
clarinet, saxophone and bassoon in certain contexts of LOOPS mean that there
are different perceptual contrast relationships in different musical
situations. The distinctiveness of a
timbre, and therefore its contrast potential, is different in different
registers (or even different pitches) in a non-simple way. we cannot think
merely in terms of gross contrast - clarinet, trumpet, saxophone, etc., but
most always consider the timbre of the instrument at whatever particular pitch
it is playing. Now, if certain
instruments can be composed in such a way that either they can be made to tend
toward homogeneity or confusability of sound or toward diversity and
distinctiveness of sound, then there is a possibility for a structural
interplay between timbre and pitch, and that is the most important musical
insight to be gained from LOOPS.
----------------------
I wish
to thank the LOOPS players, who rehearsed a difficult musical score for many
hours over a period of months. They
included Mel Warner, clarinet, Edwin Harkins, trumpet, Jean-Charles Francois,
marimba and Ron Grun, bassoon, from the Project for
Music Experiment, UCSD, and Larry Livingston alto saxophone and Peter
Middleton, flute, from the Department of Music, UCSD. Charles White, from the Project for Music
Experiment, was the recordist.
Encinitas,
Bibliography
Bregman,
A.S. and Campbell, J. (1971): "Primary Auditory Stream Segregation and
Perception of Order in Rapid Sequences of Tones," Journal of
Experimental Psychology Vol. 89, no.2, 244-249.
Miller,
B.A. and Heise, G.A. (1950): "The Trill
Threshold," Journal of the Acoustical Society of
Norman,
D.A. (1966): "Rhythmic Fission: Observations On
Attention, Temporal Judgements and the Critical
Band," unpublished ms.
Norman,
D.A. (1967): "Temporal Confusions and Limited Capactiy
Processors, Acta Psychologica,
Vol.27, 293-297.
Rufer,
J. (1969): "Noch
Einmal Schoenbergs
Op.16,"
Schoenberg,
A. (1911): Harmonielehre,
U.E. No.3370 Leipzig/Wien, p.471.